Motivation and Good Language Learners



UNIT 1
Motivation and Good Language Learners

A.    Definition of Motivation
At almost goes without saying that good language learners are motivated. Common sense and everyday experience suggest that the high achievers of this world have motivation, a word which derives from the Latin verb “movere” meaning to move. Thus, symply define, we say that motivation concerns what moves a person to make certain choices, to engage in action, and to persist in action.
Getting a dictionary to arrive at the definition of motivation is not enough to fully comprehend the definition of the word and to understand the things that it entails. Human motivation seems like a very common word, but the truth is, it can get quite complicated to grasp. In any way, it is something that can be of high value especially when it comes to success.
Motivation is one of the factors that are critical in accomplishing a set of goals. There are many hindrances that can get in the way of achieving these. This is why behavior, desires, and emotions must be in control to succeed, but how is that possible if motivation is not even clearly understood ?
Motivation is the desire that fuels a person to do certain things based on the wants and needs of a person. If a person wishes to meet these wants and needs, then it necessitates him to motivate himself so that he can make certain moves. A person is unique and apart from another so his wants and needs vary greatly from that of the other. The same is true for the people's levels of motivation.
The different wants and needs in every person vary in intensity based on the focus, goals, and the total human psyche of the person. This is why the motivation of a person is unique despite having similar wants and needs with others.


B.     Developing of Motivation
The decision to increase your motivation is the key to the bringing a greater sense of it for yourself. This can be done by putting greater focus on the things that you want to achieve until you actually accomplish them. The stronger your focus is on the goal, the greater the motivation will be. The truth of the matter is if you are unable to keep track of your goals, it can be generalized that you do not really desire to achieve it. Motivation can come naturally, and no amount of inspirational talk will set you to action if you really are not into taking the goal seriously. Setting goals that stir positive feelings within you are important to keep yourself motivated. Once you have found these, focus yourself on being able to get a hold of them. There will be a lot of distractions so pick up at a comfortable pace and make things happen. The definition of motivation involves the total understanding of the self. Receiving rewards is one reason for being motivated, but beyond that is a set of benefits that are more valuable than concrete rewards. These can be enhanced work output, more productivity, better work habits, and an increased of understanding of yourself.

C.    Kinds of Motivation
If you are trying hard to finish work so that you could beat the deadline set by your boss, then you are pushed by extrinsic motivation. On the other hand, if you are working so hard so that you could develop into a better employee, then that is intrinsic motivation. Anything that comes from within the individual is considered intrinsic, and the opposite goes for extrinsic. So, there are two kinds of motivation, they are intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation.
1.      Intrinsic Motivation
Intrinsic motivation refers to motivation that is driven by an interest or enjoyment in the task itself, and exists within the individual rather than relying on external pressures or a desire for reward. Intrinsic motivation has been studied since the early 1970s. Students who are intrinsically motivated are more likely to engage in the task willingly as well as work to improve their skills, which will increase their capabilities. Students are likely to be intrinsically motivated if they :
a.     attribute their educational results to factors under their own control, also known as autonomy.
b.     believe they have the skills to be effective agents in reaching their desired goals, also known as self-efficacy beliefs.
c.     are interested in mastering a topic, not just in achieving good grades.

2.      Extrinsic Motivation
Extrinsic motivation refers to the performance of an activity in order to attain an outcome, whether or not that activity is also intrinsically motivated. Extrinsic motivation comes from outside of the individual. Common extrinsic motivations are rewards (for example money or grades) for showing the desired behavior, and the threat of punishment following misbehavior. Competition is in an extrinsic motivator because it encourages the performer to win and to beat others, not simply to enjoy the intrinsic rewards of the activity. A cheering crowd and the desire to win a trophy are also extrinsic incentives.

Extrinsic and intrinsic motivation can also play an important role in learning settings. Some experts argue that the traditional emphasis on external rewards such as grades, report cards, and gold stars undermines any existing intrinsic motivation that students might have. Others suggest that these extrinsic motivators help students feel more competent in the classroom, thus enhancing intrinsic motivation.
"A person's interest often survives when a reward is used neither to bribe nor to control but to signal a job well done, as in a "most improved player" award. If a reward boosts your feeling of competence after doing good work, your enjoyment of the task may increase. Rewards, rightly administered, can motivate high performance and creativity. And extrinsic rewards (such as scholarships, admissions, and jobs that often follow good grades) are here to stay," explains David G. Meyers in his text Psychology: Eighth Edition in Modules[1].
As you have seen, extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motivation are both important ways of driving behavior. In order to comprehend how these can be best utilized, it is important to understand some of the key differences between the two types of motivation including the overall impact that each can have on behavior.

D.    Components of Motivation

There are three major components to motivation: activation, persistence and intensity. Activation involves the decision to initiate a behavior, such as enrolling in a psychology class. Persistence is the continued effort toward a goal even though obstacles may exist, such as taking more psychology courses in order to earn a degree although it requires a significant investment of time, energy and resources. Finally, intensity can be seen in the concentration and vigor that goes into pursuing a goal. For example, one student might coast by without much effort, while another student will study regularly, participate in discussions and take advantage of research opportunities outside of class.

E.     Motivation for the Teaching/Learning Situation
Motivation is of particular interest to educational psychologists because of the crucial role it plays in student learning. However, the specific kind of motivation that is studied in the specialized setting of education differs qualitatively from the more general forms of motivation studied by psychologists in other fields.
Motivation in education can have several effects on how students learn and how they behave towards subject matter. It can :
1.      Direct behavior toward particular goals
2.      Lead to increased effort and energy
3.      Increase initiation of, and persistence in, activities
4.      Enhance cognitive processing
5.      Determine what consequences are reinforcing
6.      Lead to improved performance.

Because students are not always internally motivated, they sometimes need situated motivation, which is found in environmental conditions that the teacher creates. If teachers decided to extrinsically reward productive student behaviors, they may find it difficult to extricate themselves from that path. Consequently student dependency on extrinsic rewards represents one of the greatest detractors from their use in the classroom.
The majority of new student orientation leaders at colleges and universities recognize that distinctive needs of students should be considered in regard to orientation information provided at the beginning of the higher education experience. Research done by Whyte in 1986 raised the awareness of counselors and educators in this regard. In 2007, the National Orientation Directors Association reprinted Cassandra B. Whyte's research report allowing readers to ascertain improvements made in addressing specific needs of students over a quarter of a century later to help with academic success[2].






F.     How to Get Motivated
Motivation is literally the desire to do things. It's the difference between waking up before dawn to pound the pavement and lazing around the house all day. It's the crucial element in setting and attaining goals and research shows you can influence your own levels of motivation and self-control. So figure out what you want, power through the pain period, and start being who you want to be.



















UNIT 2
Age and Good Language Learners

The issue of age was first addressed with the critical period hypothesis. The strict version of this hypothesis states that there is a cut-off age at about 12, after which learners lose the ability to fully learn a language. This strict version has since been rejected for second-language acquisition, as adult learners have been observed who reach native-like levels of pronunciation and general fluency. However, in general, adult learners of a second-language rarely achieve the native-like fluency that children display, despite often progressing faster in the initial stages. This has led to speculation that age is indirectly related to other, more central factors that affect language learning.
Health is an important factor in all learning, and many chronic diseases can affect the ability of the elderly to learn. Hearing loss affects many people as they age and can affect a person's ability to understand speech, especially in the presence of background noise. Visual acuity also decreases with age. (Hearing and vision problems are not restricted exclusively to the older learner, however.) It is important that the classroom environment compensate for visual or auditory impairments by combining audio input with visual presentation of new material, good lighting, and elimination of outside noise.

A.      The Role of Age in Learning Foreign Languages
If you’ve gone through puberty already, you’ll no longer be able to speak a foreign language perfectly.” This and other blanket statements are not the only thing disputed among brain researchers and linguists. But they agree on one thing: Children learn languages differently than adults. “Good morning, boys and girls!” For decades this was how English class started for most fifth graders in Germany. They were the first bits of vocabulary and basic phrases. For years now, however, increasing numbers of primary schools in Germany as well as even Kindergartens and daycare facilities are exposing younger children to their first foreign language. In order to better prepare toddlers for a multilingual future, parents, educators and policymakers are rallying support behind the concept of “the earlier the better” when it comes to learning foreign languages. But does this necessarily mean that small children in general learn foreign languages better than adolescents or adults ?.
B.       Are Children Better than Adults at Learning Languages ?
The greatest obstacle to older adult language learning is the doubt--in the minds of both learner and teacher--that older adults can learn a new language. Most people assume that "the younger the better" applies in language learning. However, many studies have shown that this is not true. Studies comparing the rate of second language acquisition in children and adults have shown that although children may have an advantage in achieving native-like fluency in the long run, adults actually learn languages more quickly than children in the early stages (Krashen, Long, and Scarcella, 1979)[3]. These studies indicate that attaining a working ability to communicate in a new language may actually be easier and more rapid for the adult than for the child.
Studies on aging have demonstrated that learning ability does not decline with age. If older people remain healthy, their intellectual abilities and skills do not decline. Adults learn differently from children, but no age-related differences in learning ability have been demonstrated for adults of different ages.


C.      The Difference between Children Learning with Adults Learning
Children often respond to new languages with curiosity and impartiality while adults increasingly look to their own learning and life experiences to help them learn the words, structures and concepts of a foreign language. The result is that children learn more through games and gestures while adults take a more analytical approach with stricter rules. Grotjahn thinks that good foreign language courses should take these various requirements and methods into account. Either way, whether you are able to sing along with Frère Jaques or La Bamba, greet the waiter on your vacation in Polish or Turkish, enjoy Chinese children’s books, or read Russian novels, the joy of learning a foreign language is something both young and old can experience to equal degrees.
This study examines the relationship between certain aspects of the second language acquisition process and age. An oral production test was developed to assess the ability of nonnative English speaking children to produce standard English morphology and syntax. The test was administered to approximately 200 children (ages 6–15) who were learning English as a second language in American public schools. The results of this testing were used to examine the relationship between age and 1) the rate of acquisition of certain English grammatical structures and 2) the order of acquisition of these grammatical structures.
The results indicated that there was some relationship between age and rate of learning. Among children exposed to English the same amounts of time, the older children scored higher on the morphology and syntax subtests, whereas the younger children received higher ratings in phonology. There were, however, no major differences observed in the order in which children of different ages learned to produce the structures included in the test. These results suggest that there is a difference in the rate of learning of English morphology, syntax and phonology based upon differences in age, but that the order of acquisition in second language learning does not change with age.
D.      Age No Excuse for Failing to Learn a Language
IT'S never too late to learn another language. Surprisingly, under controlled conditions adults turn out to be better than children at acquiring a new language skill. It is widely believed that children younger than 7 are good at picking up new languages because their brains rewire themselves more easily, and because they use what is called procedural, or implicit, memory to learn - meaning they pick up a new language without giving it conscious thought. Adults are thought to rely on explicit memory, whereby they actively learn the rules of a language. But some linguists now question whether this apparent difference in language-learning ability reflects our attitudes to young children and adults rather than differences in the brain. "If adults make a mistake we don't correct them because we don't want to insult them,".
Ferman and Avi Karni from the University of Haifa, Israel, devised an experiment in which 8-year-olds, 12-year-olds and adults were given the chance to learn a new language rule[4]. In the made-up rule, verbs were spelled and pronounced differently depending on whether they referred to an animate or inanimate object.
Participants were not told this, but were asked to listen to a list of correct noun-verb pairs, and then voice the correct verb given further nouns. The researchers had already established that 5-year-olds performed poorly at the task, and so did not include them in the study. All participants were tested again two months later to see what they remembered. "The adults were consistently better in everything we measured," says Ferman. When asked to apply the rule to new words, the 8-year-olds performed no better than chance, while most 12-year-olds and adults scored over 90 per cent. Adults fared best, and have great potential for learning new languages implicitly, says Ferman. Unlike the younger children, most adults and 12-year-olds worked out the way the rule worked - and once they did, their scores soared. This shows that explicit learning is also crucial, says Ferman, who presented the results at the International Congress for the Study of Child Language in Montreal, Canada, this week. The results are exciting, says David Birdsong from the University of Texas, Austin - particularly the finding that children's pronunciation is inferior to that of older subjects. But, Robert DeKeyser at the University of Maryland in College Park warns that artificial experiments like this do not necessarily transfer to the real world. Even if adults are better at implicit learning, children are more likely to get the chance to learn implicitly.

















REFERENCES







[1] www.goethe.de/ges/spa/pan/spg/en7142444.htm
[2] www.psychology.about.com/od/mindex/g/motivation-definition.htm
[3] www.psychology.about.com.age-related-in-language-learning.htm
[4] www.learning-acquisition-in-language-learning.htm